Empirical approach of studying individual risk preference
Boyan Markov
How likely are you to risk?
Would you pay 500$ to bet on a coin flip?
Would you risk losing 2000$ at 50/50�Or would you rather lose 1000$.
Would you risk 50/50 to gain 2000$ �or take a sure deal for 1000$.
How it all started - are you willing to risk?
I believe I am very risk averse:
1. Buy
2. Sell
3. Buy
4. Wait for breakeven
High
Low
2
Motivation behind studying risk
Some basic risk concepts
Prospect Theory Value Function
The Value Function in Prospect Theory, represented as an S-shaped graph, illuminates the psychological values assigned to losses and gains, and not simply their monetary worth.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291.
Decision making as a component of risk
Some basic risk concepts
Risk
Uncertainty
Using questionnaires to access risk
We used a Risk Preference Questionnaire (RPQ), designed to probe into the complexities of individual financial risk tolerance. Our tool uses hypothetical scenarios, placing the respondent at a crossroads between certain outcomes and risky propositions.
Key Features:
Goal: Uncover a nuanced understanding of risk preference, moving beyond the binary concept of risk-averse and risk-seeking behavior.
Publishing pending
Advantages and Disadvantages of Questionnaires and Online Surveys
Advantages
Disadvantages
Demographics of the Study: A Comprehensive Overview
Demographics of the Study: A Comprehensive Overview
Deciphering the Risk Preference Questionnaire: An In-Depth Look at the Questions
Visualizing Risk Preference: The Risk Score Graph
(n = 825)
High risk propensity sample
Key Findings: Demographics, Question Correlations, and Influencing Factors
Our analysis revealed several intriguing findings:
1. Absence of Strong Correlations with Demographics: ��Contrary to literature expectations, we found no strong correlations between our risk metric and demographics data. Typically, risk tolerance is expected to decrease with age, be higher among males, and increase with higher education and income levels.
2. Non-correlation with the First Half of Questions: ��The first 12 questions of our questionnaire showed no significant correlation with our risk metric. This may suggest that these questions are not capturing aspects of risk preference effectively or are tapping into other dimensions not reflected in our risk metric.
These findings highlight areas for potential refinement in our questionnaire and provide interesting directions for further analysis and research into the multifaceted nature of risk preference.
1.
3.
2.
Applying Cronbach's Alpha: Assessing Internal Consistency
Upon discovering a lack of correlation between the first half of the questions and our risk metric, we turn to Cronbach's Alpha. This statistical tool allows us to assess the internal consistency or reliability of our questionnaire.
What is Cronbach's Alpha?
Why Use Cronbach's Alpha in Our Context?
Through this method, we aim to ensure the integrity and efficacy of our questionnaire in capturing the multifaceted nature of risk preference.
Deciphering the Components of Cronbach's Alpha
Cronbach's Alpha (α), sometimes referred to as tau-equivalent reliability (ρ_T), hinges on several key components:
Cronbach's alpha provides an estimate of the proportion of the total score variance that is due to the true score variance, relative to the total score variance. Higher values of alpha indicate a greater proportion of reliable variance in the measure, suggesting higher internal consistency.
Applying Cronbach's Alpha: Assessing the First Six Questions
Internal consistency of first 6 questions - 0.62��Q6 has the least internal consistency and removing it would improve the metric by .04
Respondents Behavior Q1-Q6
Applying Cronbach's Alpha: Assessing the Second pair of Questions
Internal consistency of first 6 questions - 0.67��Q12 has the least internal consistency and removing it would improve the metric by .01
Respondents Behavior Q7-Q12
Applying Cronbach's Alpha: Assessing the Third pair of Questions
Internal consistency of first 6 questions - 0.17��Respondents likely did not understand the question and gave extremely nuanced answers.
Respondents Behavior Q13-Q18
Applying Cronbach's Alpha: Assessing the Last pair of Questions
Internal consistency of first 6 questions - 0.18��Respondents likely did not understand the question and gave extremely nuanced answers.
Respondents Behavior Q19-Q24
Preliminary Observations: Questionnaire Consistency and Respondent consistency
Upon applying Cronbach's Alpha, our observations reveal interesting insights about the internal consistency of our questionnaire and respondent behavior:
����
Reworking Question Values: A Categorical Approach
In an attempt to address the challenges observed in the latter half of the questionnaire, we are reworking the values of questions 13-24 into categorical results. Here is how:
For Questions 13-18: Responses will be categorized based on their relation to the expected value.
For Questions 19-24: Responses will also be categorized based on their relation to the expected loss.
By transforming these responses into categorical variables, we aim to enhance the clarity of data interpretation. Following these changes, we will reassess the internal consistency using Cronbach's Alpha to evaluate the improvement in questionnaire reliability.
Post-Transformation Cronbach's Alpha: Evaluating Questionnaire Reliability
0.17
0.87
Q13-Q18
Q13-Q18 Transformed
0.18
0.88
Q19-Q24
Q19-Q24 Transformed
Enhanced Reliability: Cronbach's Alpha Post-Transformation
After transforming the last 12 questions into categorical results, we've re-evaluated our questionnaire using Cronbach's Alpha.
Achieving Improved Consistency:
What Does this Mean?
Optimal Questionnaire Structure: Highest Internal Consistency
Our study reveals that certain combinations of questions yield superior internal consistency, as measured by Cronbach's Alpha:
Key Insights:
Potential for Shortening - These results indicate that the questionnaire could potentially be shortened without compromising its ability to assess risk preference effectively.
Stronger Signal for Risk Seeking - Our data suggests that risk aversion tends to be a stronger signal for a common construct than risk seeking among respondents, a finding that may guide future questionnaire design and interpretation.
These findings will be instrumental in refining our questionnaire design and understanding of risk preference going forward.
Challenges of Using Questionnaires in Strategic Management
Respondents get easily confused, can’t comprehend different stakes and display high variance to risk propensity.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291.
Testing the Unidimensionality of Risk Perception: An Empirical Study Using Cronbach's Alpha on Online Surveys (Markov, B., Marchev, A. 2023) *currently under review
*Preliminary findings
Game-based Approaches for Risk Propensity Calculation
Interactive and Engaging: Game-based methods can engage participants in a more immersive and dynamic way than traditional surveys or questionnaires, potentially leading to more authentic responses.�
Simulating Real-world Decisions: Games can simulate complex real-world situations where risky decisions must be made, capturing nuances that a simple questionnaire might miss.�
Dynamic Decision Making: Unlike static questionnaires, games allow for dynamic decision-making, where one decision can influence subsequent choices and outcomes.�
Quantifiable Measures of Risk Propensity: Games can provide quantifiable metrics such as points or win/lose outcomes, which can be used to calculate a participant's risk propensity based on their decision-making in the game.�
Controlled Environment: Games provide a controlled environment where researchers can manipulate variables to study their impact on risk-taking behaviors.
Methodology - key design aspects of a risk propensity game
Advantages of Online Implementation in Game-Based Risk Propensity Assessment
Modelling individual risk propensity based on decision distributions
Game-based risk assessment has practical applications and implications for strategic management practitioners. It provides a realistic and engaging approach to understand risk perception and decision-making behaviours, enabling the development of tailored risk management strategies.
Preliminary findings
Balloon analog risk task (Lejuez, 2002) 1/3
Balloon analog risk task (Lejuez, 2002) /3
Balloon analog risk task (Lejuez, 2002) 3/3
Lejuez’s approach to risk estimation
Surveying respondents | Average Clicks on Collected Bags |
Smoking and alcohol consumptions questions | High correlation |
Extreme sport practices | High correlation |
Own risk perception | High correlation |
Risk Decision-making in Bag Inflation: A Study Involving Professors, Lecturers, and Students from Three Universities
BART Core Game Loop
Source: Own
Game Evolutions - the more you play the more variance we observe
Source: Own
Bart Model Pipeline
Modeling Pipeline
Modeling Pipeline
Data | Accuracy | Precision | Recall | ROC |
Train | 76.13% | 35.87% | 80.54% | 77.96% |
Test | 73.17% | 31.54% | 71.00% | 72.27% |
Risk KPI estimation
Geometric average of every risk decision
Propensity distribution
Methodology for risk estimation
The conclusion that can be drawn is as follows:
A) The distribution itself is not normal.
and/or
B) More respondents are needed to achieve a normal distribution, suggesting that there might be a biased sample in the available data.
We confirmed hypothesis from the literature
We can’t prove that our method is normally distributed
Conclusions: Reflecting on Our Journey and Looking Ahead
Advantages of Game-based Risk Assessment:
Role of Machine Learning:
Enhances understanding of risk perception and decision-making behaviors