Chapter 10: �Perceiving Depth and Size
Lecturer: Mark Berg
Text: Goldstein 9th Ed
Overview of Questions
Figure 10.1 (a) The house is farther away than the tree, but (b) the images of points F on the house and N on the tree both fall on the two-dimensional surface of the retina, so (c) these two points, considered by themselves, do not tell us the distances of the house and the tree.
Cue Approach to Depth Perception
Cue Approach to Depth Perception - continued
Figure 10.2 (a) Convergence of the eyes occurs when a person looks at something that is very close. (b) The eyes look straight ahead when the person observes something that is far away.
Cue Approach to Depth Perception - continued
Pictorial Cues
A scene in Tucson, Arizona containing a number of depth cues: occlusion (the cactus occludes the hill, which occludes the mountain); perspective convergence (the sides of the road converge in the distance); relative size (the far motorcycle is smaller than the near one); and relative height (the far motorcycle is higher in the field of view; the far cloud is lower).
Figure 10.4 Drawings of the stimuli used in Epstein’s (1965) familiar-size experiment. The actual stimuli were photographs that were all the same size as the real quarter.
Pictorial Cues - continued
Figure 8.5 A scene along the coast of California that illustrates atmospheric perspective.
Figure 8.6 A texture gradient in Death Valley, California.
(a) Occlusion indicates that the tapered glass is in front of the round glass and vase. (b) Overlap now indicates that the vase is in front of the tapered glass, but there is something strange about this picture. (c) The cast shadow under the vase provides additional information about its position in space, which helps clear up the confusion.
Range of effectiveness of different depth cues
Motion-Produced Cues
Figure 10.8 Eye moving past (a) a nearby tree; (b) a far-away house. Notice how the image of the tree moves farther on the retina than the image of the house.
Binocular Depth Information
Binocular Depth Information - continued
Figure 8.16 The two images of a stereoscopic photograph. The difference between the two images, such as the distances between the front cactus and the window in the two views, creates retinal disparity. This creates a perception of depth when (a) the left image is viewed by the left eye and (b) the right image is viewed by the right eye.
Figure 10.19 (a) A random-dot stereogram. (b)The principle for constructing the stereogram. See text for explanation.
Correspondence Problem
Depth Perception in Other Species
Depth Perception in Other Species - continued
Figure 10.22 When a bat sends out its pulses, it receives echoes from a number of objects in the environment. This figure shows the echoes received by the bat from (a) a moth located about half a meter away; (b) a tree, located about 2 meters away; and (c) a house, located about 4 meters away. The echoes from each object return to the bat at different times, with echoes from more distant objects taking longer to return. The bat locates the positions of objects in the environment by sensing how long it takes the echoes to return.
Physiology of Depth Perception
Physiology of Depth Perception - continued
Disparity tuning curve for a neuron sensitive to absolute disparity. This curve indicates the neural response that occurs when stimuli presented the left and right eyes create different amounts of disparity. From Uka, T., & DeAngelis, G. C. (2003). Contribution of middle temporal area to coarse depth discrimination: Comparison of neuronal and psychophysical sensitivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 3515-3530.
Size Constancy
Figure 10.28 (a) The visual angle depends on the size of the stimulus (the woman in this example) and its distance from the observer. (b) When the woman moves closer to the observer, the visual angle and the size of the image on the retina increase. This example shows how halving the distance between the stimulus and observer doubles the size of the image on the retina.
Figure 10.29 The “thumb” method of determining the visual angle of an object. When the thumb is at arm’s length, whatever it covers has a visual angle of about 2 degrees. The woman’s thumb covers the width of her iPod, so the visual angle, from the woman’s view, is 2 degrees. Note that the visual angle will change as the distance between the woman and the iPod changes.
Figure 10.32 The moon’s disk almost exactly covers the sun during an eclipse because the sun and the moon have the same visual angle.
Size Constancy
Size-Distance Scaling
Figure 10.33 The principle behind the observation that the size of an afterimage increases as the afterimage is viewed against more distant surfaces.
Texture Gradient
Two cylinders resting on a texture gradient. According to Gibson (1950), the fact that the bases of both cylinders cover the same number of units on the gradient indicates that the bases of the two cylinders are the same size.
Visual Illusions
Figure 10.36 The Müller-Lyer illusion. Both lines are actually the same length.
Müller-Lyer Illusion
Müller-Lyer Illusion - continued
According to Gregory (1973), the Müller-Lyer line on the left corresponds to an outside corner, and the line on the right to an inside corner. Note that the two vertical lines are the same length (measure them!).
Figure 10.38 The “dumbbell” version of the Müller-Lyer illusion. As in the original Müller-Lyer illusion, the two lines are actually the same length.
Figure 10.39 A three-dimensional Müller-Lyer illusion.The 2-foot-high wooden “fins” stand on the floor. Although the distances x and y are the same, distance y appears larger, just as in the two-dimensional Müller-Lyer illusion.
Müller-Lyer Illusion - continued
An alternate version of the Müller-Lyer illusion. We perceive that the distance between the dots in (a) is less than the distance in (b), even though the distances are the same. (From Day, 1989.)
Ponzo Illusion
Figure 8.39 The Ponzo (or railroad track) illusion. The two horizontal rectangles are the same length on the page (measure them), but the far one appears larger.
The Ames Room
Figure 10.42 The Ames room. Both women are actually the same height, but the woman on the right appears taller because of the distorted shape of the room.
Figure 10.43 The Ames room, showing its true shape. The woman on the left is actually almost twice as far away from the observer as the one on the right; however, when the room is viewed through the peephole, this difference in distance is not seen. In order for the room to look normal when viewed through the peephole, it is necessary to enlarge the left side of the room.
The Ames Room - continued
The Ames Room - continued
Moon Illusion
Moon Illusion - continued
An artist’s conception of how the moon is perceived when it is on the horizon and when it is high in the sky. Note that the visual angle of the horizon moon is depicted as larger than the visual angle of the moon high in the sky. This is because the picture is simulating the illusion. In the environment, the visual angle of the two moons are the same.
Figure 10.45 When observers are asked to consider that the sky is a surface and to compare the distance to the horizon (H) and the distance to the top of the sky on a clear moonless night, they usually say that the horizon appears farther away. This results in the “flattened heavens” shown above.
Infant Depth Perception
Infant Depth Perception
Effects of Person’s Ability to Take Action on Distance Perception
Effects of Person’s Ability to Take Action on Distance Perception - continued
Effects of Person’s Ability to Take Action on Distance Perception - continued
Figure 10.47 Results of Witt et al.’s (2004) experiment. See text for explanation.