1 of 56

NEU 314: Mathematical Tools for Neuroscience

Lecture 3: September 15, 2022

Instructor: Sam Nastase

Princeton Neuroscience Institute

Trichromatic color vision

2 of 56

3 of 56

Maxwell’s color-matching experiment

Any “test” light can be matched by adjusting the intensities of any three other lights

4 of 56

Visible light

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

5 of 56

Visible light

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

6 of 56

Visible light

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

How many numbers would you need to write down to specify the color of a light source?

Just one? The wavelength?

620 nm?

7 of 56

Visible light

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

How many numbers would you need to write down to specify the color of a light source?

spectral properties

8 of 56

Visible light

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

How many numbers would you need to write down to specify the color of a light source?

spectral properties

9 of 56

Visible light

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

How many numbers would you need to write down to specify the color of a light source?

spectral properties

10 of 56

Visible light

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

How many numbers would you need to write down to specify the color of a light source?

spectral properties

11 of 56

Visible light

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

How many numbers would you need to write down to specify the color of a light source?

spectral properties

12 of 56

Visible light

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

How many numbers would you need to write down to specify the color of a light source?

spectral properties

13 of 56

Color vision

“Now, as it is almost impossible to conceive each sensitive point of the retina to contain an infinite number of particles, each capable of vibrating in perfect unison with every possible undulation, it is necessary to suppose the number limited, for instance, to the three principal colours, red, yellow, and blue, of which the undulates are related in magnitude.”

Young, 1805

14 of 56

Color vision

“Now, as it almost impossible to conceive each sensitive point of the retina to contain an infinite number of particles, each capable of vibrating in perfect unison with every possible undulation, it is necessary to suppose the number limited, for instance, to the three principal colours, red, yellow, and blue, of which the undulates are related in magnitude.”

Maxwell, 1864

Young, 1802

Helmholtz, 1850

15 of 56

How many measurements of this spectrum does the human eye take?

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

Color vision

16 of 56

How many measurements of this spectrum does the human eye take?

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

Only three! One for each cone (i.e. color-sensitive photoreceptor cell) class:

photoreceptor response

S cones: short wavelength (blue)

M cones: medium wavelength (green)

L cones: long wavelength (red)

Color vision

17 of 56

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

Human color vision relies entirely on the comparison of responses from three cone types!

photoreceptor response

S cones: short wavelength (blue)

M cones: medium wavelength (green)

L cones: long wavelength (red)

Color vision

18 of 56

Visible light is transduced by photoreceptor cells (rods and cones) in the retina

A brief tour of early vision in humans

19 of 56

Visible light is transduced by photoreceptor cells (rods and cones) in the retina

A brief tour of early vision in humans

20 of 56

Visible light is transduced by photoreceptor cells (rods and cones) in the retina

A brief tour of early vision in humans

Rods are sensitive in low-light conditions, are more prevalent in the periphery, and contain only one pigment type

Cones require bright light, are more prevalent in the fovea, and contain three different pigment types

21 of 56

Visible light is transduced by photoreceptor cells (rods and cones) in the retina

A brief tour of early vision in humans

Rods are sensitive in low-light conditions, are more prevalent in the periphery, and contain only one pigment type

Cones require bright light, are more prevalent in the fovea, and contain three different pigment types

Photoreceptors are tonically depolarized; when pigment proteins (opsins) absorb photons, the cell becomes hyperpolarized

22 of 56

Visible light is transduced by photoreceptor cells (rods and cones) in the retina

A brief tour of early vision in humans

Photoreceptors synapse on bipolar cells, which become depolarized or hyper- polarized depending on the receptor type

Two populations of bipolar cells: ON bipolar cells are excited by light; OFF bipolar cells are inhibited by light

Other cell types in the plexiform layers (e.g. horizontal cells) provide additional preprocessing; e.g. lateral inhibition

23 of 56

Visible light is transduced by photoreceptor cells (rods and cones) in the retina

A brief tour of early vision in humans

Photoreceptors synapse on bipolar cells, which become depolarized or hyper- polarized depending on the receptor type

Two populations of bipolar cells: ON bipolar cells are excited by light; OFF bipolar cells are inhibited by light

Other cell types in the plexiform layers (e.g. horizontal cells) provide additional preprocessing; e.g. lateral inhibition

This is where we start setting the stage for center-surround receptive fields and opponent processing!

24 of 56

Visible light is transduced by photoreceptor cells (rods and cones) in the retina

A brief tour of early vision in humans

Retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) receive input from bipolar cells and send action potentials to the brain via the optic nerve

25 of 56

A brief tour of early vision in humans

Livingstone & Hubel, 1988

Signals from the RGCs are sent to the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) in the thalamus, then on to primary visual cortex (V1)

The parvocellular pathway is color-selective, slow, high- resolution (small receptive fields)

The magnocellular pathway is color-blind, motion-sensitive, low- resolution (large receptive fields)

26 of 56

A brief tour of early vision in humans

Signals from the RGCs are sent to the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) in the thalamus, then on to primary visual cortex (V1)

The parvocellular pathway is color-selective, slow, high- resolution (small receptive fields)

The magnocellular pathway is color-blind, motion-sensitive, low- resolution (large receptive fields)

27 of 56

Visible light is just a thin slice of the electromagnetic spectrum: ~380 to ~750 nm

Human color vision relies entirely on the comparison of responses from three cone types!

photoreceptor response

S cones: short wavelength (blue)

M cones: medium wavelength (green)

L cones: long wavelength (red)

Color vision

28 of 56

Responses from a single cone type are ambiguous! (⊙_◎)

photoreceptor response

The problem of univariance

Absorption spectrum describes the response of a photoreceptor as a function of wavelength

29 of 56

Responses from a single cone type are ambiguous! (⊙_◎)

photoreceptor response

The problem of univariance

response ≈ .5

Absorption spectrum describes the response of a photoreceptor as a function of wavelength

30 of 56

Responses from a single cone type are ambiguous! (⊙_◎)

photoreceptor response

The problem of univariance

response ≈ .5

Absorption spectrum describes the response of a photoreceptor as a function of wavelength

31 of 56

Responses from a single cone type are ambiguous! (⊙_◎)

photoreceptor response

The problem of univariance

response ≈ .5

Photoreceptor has the same response to both of these wavelengths!

But the problem gets even worse…

Absorption spectrum describes the response of a photoreceptor as a function of wavelength

32 of 56

Responses from a single cone type are ambiguous! (⊙_◎)

photoreceptor response

The problem of univariance

response ≈ .5

The photoreceptor response scales with light intensity! Can’t differentiate weak intensity at peak absorption and high intensity at off-peak absorption

Absorption spectrum describes the response of a photoreceptor as a function of wavelength

×.5

×2

33 of 56

An infinite set of wavelength–intensity combinations can elicit the same response!

photoreceptor response

The problem of univariance

response ≈ .5

×.5

×2

34 of 56

Three-dimensional color space

We can compute 3D cone response vectors using matrix-vector multiplication

35 of 56

Three-dimensional color space

We can compute 3D cone response vectors using matrix-vector multiplication

illuminant spectrum

cone absorption spectra

cone responses

36 of 56

Maxwell’s color-matching experiment

Any “test” light (vector) can be matched by adjusting the intensities of any three other lights (basis vectors)

37 of 56

Metamers

Metamers are illuminants that are physically distinct but are perceptually indistinguishable

Metamers are a subspace of illuminants that have the same linear projection onto the three cone absorption spectra basis vectors!

38 of 56

Metamers

Metamers are illuminants that are physically distinct but are perceptually indistinguishable

illuminant spectrum

cone absorption spectrum

cone responses

39 of 56

Three-dimensional color space

Maxwell created the first color photograph using a composite of photographs with red, green, and blue filters

Maxwell, 1861

40 of 56

Three-dimensional color space

Monitors and digital images rely on only three illuminants to approximate any color

CRT monitor with three

(red, green, blue) phosphors

41 of 56

Color blindness

Dichromats typically only have two functional cone types

Protanopia: absence of L cones

Deuteranopia: absence of M cones

Tritanopia: absence of S cones

42 of 56

Color blindness

Dichromats typically only have two functional cone types

Protanopia: absence of L cones

Deuteranopia: absence of M cones

Tritanopia: absence of S cones

43 of 56

Color blindness

Dichromats typically only have two functional cone types

Protanopia: absence of L cones

Deuteranopia: absence of M cones

Tritanopia: absence of S cones

Color-blind folks are living in a two-dimensional subspace! (⌐▨_▨)

44 of 56

Color vision in other animals

There’s an incredible variety of color vision in the animal kingdom

—most mammals (dogs, cats, horses): dichromats

—old-world primates (including us): trichromats

—marine mammals: monochromats

—bees: trichromats (lack red “L” cone; ultraviolet instead)

—some birds, reptiles & amphibians: tetrachromats!

45 of 56

Color vision in other animals

There’s an incredible variety of color vision in the animal kingdom

Some tetrachromatic birds (e.g. zebra finches) use ultraviolet vision in foraging and display ultraviolet plumage during mate selection

46 of 56

Center-surround receptive fields

The receptive fields of retinal ganglion cells (and LGN cells) exhibit center-surround antagonism

47 of 56

Center-surround receptive fields

The receptive fields of retinal ganglion cells (and LGN cells) exhibit center-surround antagonism

48 of 56

Center-surround receptive fields

The receptive fields of retinal ganglion cells (and LGN cells) exhibit center-surround antagonism

49 of 56

Center-surround receptive fields

We can model this phenomenon using the dot product!

RGC

photoreceptors

50 of 56

Center-surround receptive fields

We can model this phenomenon using the dot product!

RGC

photoreceptors

51 of 56

Center-surround receptive fields

We can model this phenomenon using the dot product!

RGC

photoreceptors

52 of 56

Center-surround receptive fields

We can model this phenomenon using the dot product!

RGC

photoreceptors

53 of 56

Color constancy

54 of 56

The visual system discounts the illuminant; it cares more about the surface than the illuminant

Color constancy

55 of 56

The visual system discounts the illuminant; it cares more about the surface than the illuminant

Color constancy

But color constancy isn’t perfect! 「(゚ペ)

56 of 56

dodecachromatic color vision?