Chapter 8: �Perceiving Motion
Lecturer: Mark Berg
Text: Goldstein 9th Ed
Overview of Questions
Functions of Movement Perception
Functions of Movement Perception - continued
Four Ways to Perceive Motion
Four Ways to Perceive Motion - continued
Four ways to create the perception of movement: (a) real movement - the object moves; (b) apparent movement - dots blinking on and off create a moving message (see Figure 5.10 for the beginning of this message); (c) induced movement - the moon appears to race through the clouds; and (d) induced movement - stimulus for the waterfall illusion.
Comparison of Real and Apparent Motion
Comparison of Real and Apparent Motion - continued
Three Situations that Lead to Motion Perception
Three Situations that Lead to Motion Perception - continued
Figure 8.8 Three motion situations: (a) Maria is stationary and observes Jeremy walking past; (b) Maria follows Jeremy’s movement with her eyes; (c) Maria walks through the room.
Table 8.1 Conditions for perceiving and not perceiving motion depicted in Figure 8.8
Two Explanations of Motion Perception
Ecological Approach to Motion Perception
Moving a pencil between an aperture.
The circle represents a neuron’s receptive field. When the pencil is moved up and to the right, as shown, movement of the tip of the pencil provides information indicating that the pencil is moving up and to the right.
The pole’s overall motion is horizontally to the right (blue arrows). The circle represents the area in Maria’s field of view that corresponds to the receptive field of a cortical neuron. The pole’s motion across the receptive field (which is located on Maria’s retina) is also horizontal to the right (red arrows).
In this situation the pole’s overall motion is up and to the right (blue arrows). The pole’s motion across the receptive field is horizontal to the right (red arrows), as in Figure 8.11. Thus, the receptive field “sees” the same motion whether the overall motion is horizontal or up and to the right.
Physiological Approach to Motion Perception - continued
Figure 8.13 Human brain, showing the location of a number of the structures we will be discussing in this chapter. MT = medial temporal cortex (motion perception); MST = medial superior sulcus (motion perception); STS = superior temporal sulcus (biological motion); FFA = fusiform face area (face perception); EBA = extrastriate body area (perceiving bodies).
Determining Direction of Fields of Moving Dots
Figure 8.15 Moving-dot displays used by Newsome, Britten, and Movshon (1989). These pictures represent moving-dot displays that were created by a computer. Each dot survives for a brief interval (20-30 microseconds), after which it disappears and is replaced by another randomly placed dot. Coherence is the percentage of dots moving in the same direction at any point in time. From Newsome, W. T., & Pare, E. B. (1988). A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 2201-2211.
The perceptual cycle from Chapter 1. Newsome measured relationship PH2 by simultaneously recording from neurons and measuring the monkey’s behavioral response. Other research we have discussed, such as Hubel and Wiesel’s receptive field studies, have measured relationship PH1.
Determining Direction of Fields of Moving Dots - continued
Determining Direction of Fields of Moving Dots - continued
A monkey judges the motion of dots moving horizontally to the right. (b) When a column of neurons that prefer downward motion is stimulated, the monkey judges the same motion as being downward and to the right.
Effect of Eye Movements on Motion Perception
Physiological Approach to Movement Perception - continued
Figure 8.10 (a) When the image of an object moves across the retina, movement of the image across the retina creates an image displacement signal (IDS). (b) When a motor signal (MS) to move the eyes is sent to the eye muscles, so the eye can follow a moving object, there is a corollary discharge signal (CDS), which splits off from the motor signal.
The corollary discharge model. The motor area sends the motor signal (MS) to move the eyes to the eye muscles and sends the corollary discharge signal (CDS) to a structure called the comparator. Movement of a stimulus across the retina generates an image movement/displacement signal (IMS or IDS), which also goes to the comparator. The comparator sends its output to the visual cortex. See text for details. (From “Perception,” by H. L. Teuber, 1960. In J. Field, H. W. Magoun & V. E. Hall (Eds.) Handbook of Physiology, Section 1, Neurophysiology, Vol. 3, pp. 1595-1668, figure 31. Copyright © 1960 by the American Physiological Society. Reprinted by permission.)
Figure 8.21 When the eye moves in the dark, the image remains stationary (the bleached area on the retina), but a corollary discharge signal is sent to the comparator, so the afterimage appears to move.
In all four examples shown in the figure, a signal is sent to the eye muscles, and a corollary discharge is generated. However, no image movement/displacement signal is generated, so movement is perceived. See text for details.
Ignore the Figure references. Also the IMS = IDS
Figure 8.8 Three motion situations: (a) Maria is stationary and observes Jeremy walking past; (b) Maria follows Jeremy’s movement with her eyes; (c) Maria walks through the room.
Physiological Evidence for Corollary Discharge Theory
Figure 8.23 Responses of a real-movement neuron in a extrastriate cortex of a monkey. In both cases, a bar sweeps across the neuron’s receptive field. (a) The neuron fires when the bar moves to the left across the retina. (b) The neuron doesn’t fire when the eye moves to the right past the bar. Adapted from Galletti, C., & Fattori, P. (2003). Neuronal mechanisms for detection of motion in the field of view. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1717-1727.
Perception of Biological Motion
Figure 8.25 Frames from the stimuli used by Grossman and Blake (2001). (a) Sequence from the point-light walker stimulus. (b) Sequence from the scrambled point-light stimulus.
Perception of Biological Motion - continued
Figure 8.26 (a) Biological motion stimulus; (b) scrambled stimulus; (c) stimulus from a, with “noise” added (dots corresponding to the walker are indicated by lines, which were not seen by the observer); (d) how the stimulus appears to the observer. From Grossman, E. D., Batelli, L., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Repetitive TMS over posterior STS disrupts perception of biological motion. Vision Research, 45, 2847-2853.
Implied Motion
Experiment by Reed and Vinson
Figure 8.28 Stimuli used by Reed and Vinson (1996) to demonstrate the effect of experience on representational momentum. In this example, the test pictures are lower than the memory picture. On other trials the rocket would appear in the same position as or higher than the memory picture.
Experiment by Kourtzi and Kanwisher
Figure 8.29 Examples of pictures used by Kourtzi and Kanwisher (2000) to depict implied motion (IM), no implied motion (no-IM), at rest (R), and a house (H). The height of the bar below each picture indicates the average fMRI response of the MT cortex to that type of picture.
Apparent Motion: The Occlusion Heuristic
Figure 8.30 Stimuli from the Ramachandran and Anstis (1986) experiment. (a) “The initial stimulus condition. Both dots move to the position of the dot on the right. (b) Placing a square in the position shown changes the perception of the movement of the lower dot, which now moves to the right and under the square.