Page 1 of 16

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

Does the Priming and Valence of an Object Effect Emotional Processing when

Presented Together?

School of Education, Language and Psychology, York St John University.

Psychology BSc

Dr Matthew Coxon

21 st May 2021.

Page 2 of 16

2

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

Abstract

Previous literature has not investigated, both, priming and valence on emotional

processing. As a result, the present paper aims to fill this gap. The hypothesis proposed that

negatively charged words (negative valence) will significantly reduce the number of correct

responses when assessing perceptual processing. The paper also proposed that previously

primed words will significantly reduce the number of correct responses when assessing

perceptual processing. The study used a 2x3 within-subjects design, gathering data from 18

participants. Participants were York St John psychology students or registered on LinkedIn.

Participants who did not complete the experiment were removed. The study requested the age

and gender of the participants. Participants completed the emotional word identification task

and the implicit encoding memory task. Descriptive statistics and Levene's test were

calculated. Descriptives were at an acceptable level. Valence and priming did not violate

Mauchly’s test of sphericity, the sphericity assumed value was used. The interaction (priming

x valence) did violate Mauchly’s test of sphericity, therefore the Greenhouse–Geisser value

was used. A 2x3 within-subjects ANOVA analysed the data. The interaction was significant;

F (2,34) =13.78, p=<0.01, =0.49. Post Hoc T-tests were used with Bonferroni correction. A

significant effect was found for the negative valence condition; t (17) =-4.60, P=0.003. Future

research should conduct the previous study with participants with ADHD.

[Word Count: 2526]

Keywords: Valence, Priming, Emotional Processing, Implicit Association, Gorilla, 2x3

within-subjects ANOVA, T-Test, Bonferroni Correction.

Page 3 of 16

3

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

Introduction

This paper explores the effect of priming and valence on emotional processing when

presented together. Priming is defined as the previous stimuli affecting the response to new

stimuli (Bermeitinger, 2016). Valence is classed as the emotional charge of a word (APA,

2020). Based on the underpinning theory, valence and priming would reduce the number of

correct responses. Masked priming may reduce the number of correct words due to the

facilitation of memories, hindering the recognition of stimuli (Verywellmind, 2020). The

valence of a word makes it more memorable due to enhanced arousal (Lavoie, 2013).

Extensive research demonstrates the effect of valence on emotional processing.

Zeelenberg et al. (2006) utilised a perceptual identification task to disentangle bias from

enhanced processing. Twenty-nine participants completed 9 conditions, containing 16 trials

in each condition. The study found emotionally charged targets were identified quicker than

neutral targets. The positive or negative valence of a word influenced performance,

confirmed by a two-way ANOVA. Post-hoc Tukey indicated the foil condition had no

significant effect on performance. As a result, Zeelenberg et al. (2006) show that valence

affects emotional processing because the neutral stimuli had no significant effect. Hamman et

al. (1999) explained the results through the stronger representation of an emotionally

significant word. The alternative explanation reduces the internal validity. Also, Zeelenberg

(2006) fails to define the concept of valence. The absent definition reduces the clarity of the

research because the paper fails to define the independent variable. Vuilleumier (2005)

reviewed the evidence that used brain scans to assess how selective attention is affected by

the emotional charge of stimuli. The amygdala became increased in activity when presented

with emotionally charged stimuli. Vuilleumier (2005) shows that valence affects emotional

processing because the amygdala is associated with fear. As a result, the human survival

principle ensures stimuli that threaten a human existence becomes easily identified

Page 4 of 16

4

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

(Buchanan & Adolphs, 2002). Eckerman (1981) states that fMRI scans have low temporal

validity. As a result, fMRI may not be the most effective method to objectively measure

valence on emotional processing. Richards et al. (1999) has criticised Vuilleumier (2005) as

none of the experiments offered any support for the prediction of a threat-related implicit

memory bias in high trait anxiety, conflicting with findings of other literature. Kensinger and

Schacter (2006) found similar findings to Vuilleumier (2005). The aim was to use brain scans

to see what areas of the brain react to arousing stimuli. The researchers employed twenty-one

participants. The participants were shown positive, negative, and neutral photos, whilst under

an fMRI. The study found positive valence was associated with the prefrontal cortex.

Although, research has explored alternative explanations that may be considered more

feasible (Kauschke, Bahn, Vesker, & Schwarzer, 2019). As a result, this questions the

internal validity.

Research into the effect of priming on emotional processing is varied. Hart et al.

(2010) used MRI scans on fourteen volunteers aged 18-36 (M=25.3 years). The sample

includes five males and nine females. Participants carried out a Stroop test, containing neutral

and aversive stimuli. The images were presented on the same device and the images were the

same size. The study found previous words with emotional priming affect the recall of words.

Hart et al. (2010) show the effect of priming on emotional processing because previous

stimuli affected the emotional processing of new stimuli. Although, this effect is dependent

on the emotional significance of the stimuli. Thus, questioning whether it is priming or

valence that affects recall. Hart et al. (2010) used a small sample. As a result, a substantial

effect size may not be obtained. However, the sample included males and females. The

research is more generalisable due to males and females, instead of a one-sided sample.

Matthews and Southall (1991) investigated the effect of depression on recognition memory.

the study utilised patients with depression. 10 participants (3 men, 7 women), with long-

Page 5 of 16

5

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

standing depression, were referred to Stafford District Psychological Services. The study

employed a semantic priming paradigm, finding that depressives showed enhanced priming

of emotionally charged words. This study fails to account for the participant’s differing levels

of depression, therefore would the same results be obtained for different levels of depression.

Mathews and Southall (1991) could be improved for having different conditions for different

levels of depression. Rohr et al. (2012) found similar results to Matthew and Southall. The

aim was to investigate the specificity of emotion processing, under the condition of limited

awareness. 81 Participants (26 men, 55 women) were recruited. The experiment used a

modified affective priming task. Participants were asked to categorise the target emotion. In

all conditions, participants were affected by emotional primes. Research supports priming on

the implicit association, using pictures instead of words (Spruyt, 2002).

Rationale, Aims and Hypotheses

`Previous research investigates the effect of valence or priming on emotional

processing. The present paper uses valence and priming in the same experiment. This will

address the gap in the literature. Based on previous literature, the current paper proposes the

following hypothesis:

H 1 Negatively charged words (negative valence) will significantly reduce the number of

correct responses when assessing perceptual processing.

H 2 Previously primed words will significantly reduce the number of correct responses when

assessing perceptual processing.

Method

Design

The study used a 2x3 within-participants design. Two independent variables (IV) were

used (priming and valence condition). Valence contained three levels: positive, negative, and

Page 8 of 16

8

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

positive unprimed. Skew and kurtosis were acceptable. Although, the skew is slightly

negative.

Table 2

Descriptive Statistics

Valence Priming M SD

Negative

Prime 0.78 0.24

Unprimed 0.88 0.20

Neutral

Prime 0.85 0.20

Unprimed 0.80 0.17

Positive

Prime 0.87 0.17

Unprimed 0.81 0.17

Notes: M=Mean. SD= Standard Deviation. Dependent variable=the number of correct

responses.

ANOVA

A factorial ANOVA was conducted to compare the main effects of priming, valence,

and the interaction effect (priming x valence). There was no statistically significant main

effect on the number of correct responses due to valence; F(1.40, 34) =0.37, p=0.62,  2 =0.02.

There was no statistically significant main effect on the number of correct responses due to

priming; F(1,17)=0.03, p=0.86,  2 =0.002. There was a significant interaction between

valence and priming; F (2,34) =13.78, p=<0.01,  2 =0.49.

Page 11 of 16

11

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

by using a Bonferroni combined with the T-test because the chances of using a T-Test three

times increases the chance of error. Therefore, multiplying the significance by the number of

T-Tests accounts for the increased error, increasing the validity of the results.

Closing Remarks

The present paper found a significant effect in the interaction between priming and

valence. Primed stimuli were correctly identified more than unprimed stimuli, except for

neutral words, supporting previous research (Hart et al., 2010; Matthews and Southall, 1991).

The study has several weaknesses, including the failure to exclude participants with ADHD

(Searight, 2000). As a result, future research into the role of valence and priming in

perceptual processing should conduct the previous study with ADHD participants.

Page 12 of 16

12

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

References

American Psychological Association. (2020). Valence. APA Dictionary of Psychology.

https://dictionary.apa.org/valence

Bermeitinger, C. (2016). Psychology and mental health: Concepts, methodologies, tools, and

applications. IGI Global.

Buchanan, T. W., & Adolphs, R. (2002). The role of the human amygdala in emotional

modulation of long-term declarative memory. Advances in Consciousness Research, 44

(1), 9-34.

Eckerman, K., Cristy, M., & Warner, G. (1981). Dosimetric evaluation of brain scanning

agents. International Journal of Nuclear Medicine and Biology, 10(1), 55, 1-9

Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Grafton, S. T., & Kilts, C. D. (1999). Amygdala activity related to

enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nature neuroscience, 2(3), 289-

293.

Hart, S. J., Green, S. R., Casp, M., & Belger, A. (2010). Emotional priming effects during

Stroop task performance. Neuroimage, 49(3), 2662-2670.

Kauschke, C., Bahn, D., Vesker, M., & Schwarzer, G. (2019). The role of emotional valence

for the processing of facial and verbal stimuli—positivity or negativity bias? Frontiers

in psychology, 10(1), 1654-1654.

Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Processing emotional pictures and words: Effects

of valence and arousal. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(2), 110-

126.

Kensinger E. A. (2009). Remembering the Details: Effects of Emotion. Emotion review

journal of the International Society for Research on Emotion, 1(2), 99–113.

Page 13 of 16

13

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

Lavoie, M. E., & O'Connor, K. P. (2013). Effect of emotional valence on episodic memory

stages as indexed by event-related potentials. World journal of neuroscience, 2013(3),

250–262.

Matthews, G., & Southall, A. (1991). Depression and the processing of emotional stimuli: A

study of semantic priming. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15(4), 283-302.

National Health Service (2019). Colour Vision Deficiency: Colour Blindness.

https://www.nhs.uk/conditions/colour-vision-deficiency/

Richards, A. (1999). Implicit memory and anxiety: Perceptual identification of emotional

stimuli. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11(1), 67-86.

Rohr, M., Degner, J., & Wentura, D. (2012). Masked emotional priming beyond global

valence activations. Cognition & Emotion, 26(2), 224-244.

Searight, H. R., Burke, J. M., & Rottnek, F. R. E. D. (2000). Adult ADHD: evaluation and

treatment in family medicine. American family physician, 62(9), 2077-2086.

Spruyt, A., Hermans, D., Houwer, J. D., & Eelen, P. (2002). On the nature of the affective

priming effect: Affective priming of naming responses. Social Cognition, 20(3), 227-

256.

Stone, M., Ladd, S. L., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1998). Word-identification priming

for ignored and attended words. Consciousness and Cognition, 7(2), 238-258.

Verywellmind (2020). The Priming Process. Priming and the Psychology of Memory.

https://www.verywellmind.com/priming-and-the-psychology-of-memory-4173092#the-

priming-process.

Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional

attention. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9(12), 585-594.

Page 14 of 16

14

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

Weber, B., Kügler, F., & Elger, C. E. (2007). Comparison of implicit memory encoding

paradigms for the activation of mediotemporal structures. Epilepsy & Behavior, 10(3),

442-448.

Widdowson, H. G. (1993). Communication, community, and the problem of appropriate

use: Language, Communication and Social Meaning, Georgetown University Press.

York St John University (2020) Code of Practice and Research Integrity.

https://www.yorksj.ac.uk/media/content-

assets/research/documents/RO01_CodePracticeResIntegrity_201903_v2.docx

York St John University (2020) Research Ethics Policy.

https://www.yorksj.ac.uk/media/content-assets/research/documents/Research-Ethics-

Policy.docx

Zeelenberg, R., Wagenmakers, E. J., & Rotteveel, M. (2006). The impact of emotion on

perception: Bias or enhanced processing? Psychological Science, 17(4), 287-291.

Page 15 of 16

15

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

Appendix

Table 1

Ethical Issues

Ethical Issue Solution

Informed Consent A consent form was created and presented through

gorilla. Participants were asked if they were happy to

continue. After, the participants had to tick the boxes to

start the projects.

Protection From Harm Participants may feel worthless due to not being able to

complete the task. As a result, the present paper states

that the participant does not have to. answer the

questions if they do not want to.

Right to Withdraw Participants may not be aware that they have the right to

withdraw. Therefore, the experiment explains what will

be expected. The participants can withdraw during the

experiment, however, participants cannot withdraw their

data after the experiment. This is due to the lack of

pseudonyms, meaning data cannot be identified.

Confidentiality/Anonymity Participants were not asked for a pseudonym, therefore

the results from their participation were not linked to

their identity, making the data anonymous. Data was not

kept longer than was necessary.

Raising Queries Participants were presented with a consent form and

experiment information sheet containing the details of

Page 16 of 16

16

PRIMING, VALENCE AND PROCESSING

the researcher, the supervisor, module leader and ethics

supervisor.

Deception Participants were presented with experiment

information and a consent form, allowing for a detailed

procedure of the experiment. In addition to how the

information will be used.

Notes. Code of Practice and Research Integrity (York St John University, 2020). Research

Ethics Policy (York St John University, 2020).